ReviewEssays.com - Term Papers, Book Reports, Research Papers and College Essays
Search

The Bystander Effect

Essay by   •  February 9, 2011  •  Research Paper  •  2,640 Words (11 Pages)  •  2,979 Views

Essay Preview: The Bystander Effect

Report this essay
Page 1 of 11

There is one question that has undoubtedly crossed the minds of most Americans at one time in their life, and continues to plague the country. Should I help or should I just walk away? What I am referring to is something psychologists have named the Bystander Effect. According to the Penguin Dictionary of Psychology, the bystander effect is defined as such: the more people present when help is needed, the less likely any of them is to provide assistance (2001). At first glance this definition seems a bit backwards. Common sense leads one to believe that there is safety in numbers. However, through research and personal exposure to this phenomenon in our society, the proof of this definition is all too shocking. One well known example of this is the homicide case in New York City involving Kitty Genovese.

For roughly thirty-five minutes, thirty-eight residents in the apartments that overlooked the street watched from their windows as Kitty Genovese was brutally attacked and continually stabbed. Not a single resident offered assistance or in the least, called the police (Rosenthal 1964). Where does one begin to try and understand this careless disregard for the safety of others? This event is a perfect demonstration of the Bystander Effect. While the residents were watching the attack they also noticed that others were watching. With this knowledge of others’ awareness of the situation, a diffusion of responsibility occurs. “If others are known to be present, but their behavior cannot be closely observed, any one bystander can assume that one of the other observers is already taking action to end the emergency. Therefore, his own intervention would be only redundant---perhaps harmfully or confusingly so…thus convincing himself that вЂ?somebody else might be doing something’.” (Darley & LatanÐ"Ё 1968-1) This thought that someone else will assist the victim or that another bystander has probably called the police, enters most people’s mind at this stage. What Darley and LatanÐ"Ё also discovered was that people within their studies were worried about the shame and/or guilt that they would feel if they did not assist the victim. On the flip-side of this thought, the bystander did not want to be humiliated, embarrassed, or made a fool of (1968-1). One thing this shows is that people within our society could put their social standing or even their personal pride above a simple task as assisting a person in need. This reason is hopefully not the whole reason that a person would choose inaction, however it is involved on some level.

Most emergencies are, or at least begin as, ambiguous events. A quarrel in the street may erupt into violence, but it may be simply a family argument. A man staggering may be suffering a coronary or an onset of diabetes; he may be simply drunk. Smoke pouring from a building may signal a fire; on the other hand, it may be simply steam or air-conditioning vapor. Before a bystander is likely to take action in such ambiguous situations, he must first define the event as an emergency and decide that intervention is the proper course of action…and he must decide that it is his personal responsibility to act. (LatanÐ"Ё & Darley 1968)

Bibb LatanÐ"Ё and John Darley found that a crowd, instead of having a higher rate of assistance, can force inaction on its members (1968-2). The popular thought is that a group could interact and discuss the situation and ultimately offer more assistance to a victim than a single person could. “An individual, seeing the inaction of others, will judge a situation as less serious than he would if he were alone…Until someone acts, each person only sees the other non-responding bystanders, and is likely to be influenced not to act himself” (1968-2). The only way to completely understand these findings and their proceeding actions is for one to observe people in a situation to see how they would truly act. Irving Piliavin, Judith Rodin, and Jane Piliavin conducted a “field experiment to find the effects of several variables on helping behavior. This experiment was conducted on the express trains of the New York 8th Avenue Independent Subway” (Piliavin et. al. 1969). These experiments were conducted to be able to monitor different conditions that could possibly accompany a bystander’s decision to assist.

Evidence uncovered by the trio showed that bystanders “…will sometimes derogate the characters of the victims of misfortune, instead of feeling compassion” (1969). The thought process of this brings back the diffusion of responsibility. If one takes these findings and couples it with another experiment done by John Darley and Bibb LatanÐ"Ё which states, “…an individual may feel less personal responsibility for helping[the victim] if he shares the responsibility with others” (LatanÐ"Ё & Darley 1968), a reasonable individual will begin to realize that there is an emerging pattern of thought on the bystanders part. However, Irving Piliavin, Judith Rodin, and Jane Piliavin also found that, “Several investigators found that an individual’s actions in a given situation does lead others in that situation to engage in similar actions” (Piliavin et al 1969). What this information does reveal are people's social vulnerability to each other. The experiments findings within the Piliavin, Rodin, Piliavin study was that a person who is ill is more likely to receive aid as compared to an apparent drunk. Also, they found that the race of the victim has very little bearing on the race of the helper, except when the victim is drunk (1969). Now, when these experiments findings were originally published in 1969, racism and racial prejudice were much more visible and quite a bit more involved in the daily lives of the public. Though, to say that racism and prejudice no longer exist in America is a bald face fallacy. But we have, as a country, come very far enough along the road to equality to be able to see a visible change in that society. There is, “…very little evidence of prejudice toward sober individuals, whether white or black. There was a large increase (65%) in prejudice expressed towards drunks of both races (white and black)” (1969). So when alcohol is a factor of the victim’s situation, the possibility of help from any bystander is decreased immensely. The gender make-up of a group, surprisingly, also affects which bystander will respond. The Subway study showed that, “given mixed groups of men and women, and there is a male victim, men are more likely to help than are women”

...

...

Download as:   txt (16.3 Kb)   pdf (176.1 Kb)   docx (15.2 Kb)  
Continue for 10 more pages »
Only available on ReviewEssays.com
Citation Generator

(2011, 02). The Bystander Effect. ReviewEssays.com. Retrieved 02, 2011, from https://www.reviewessays.com/essay/The-Bystander-Effect/35913.html

"The Bystander Effect" ReviewEssays.com. 02 2011. 2011. 02 2011 <https://www.reviewessays.com/essay/The-Bystander-Effect/35913.html>.

"The Bystander Effect." ReviewEssays.com. ReviewEssays.com, 02 2011. Web. 02 2011. <https://www.reviewessays.com/essay/The-Bystander-Effect/35913.html>.

"The Bystander Effect." ReviewEssays.com. 02, 2011. Accessed 02, 2011. https://www.reviewessays.com/essay/The-Bystander-Effect/35913.html.